How do great bowerbirds construct perspective illusions?

Many animals build structures to provide shelter, avoid predation, attract mates or house offspring, but the behaviour and potential cognitive processes involved during building are poorly understood. Great bowerbird (Ptilinorhynchus nuchalis) males build and maintain display courts by placing tens...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Laura A. Kelley, John A. Endler
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: The Royal Society 2017-01-01
Series:Royal Society Open Science
Subjects:
Online Access:https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/pdf/10.1098/rsos.160661
id doaj-9c724507dda14d51a03cf8ddf3a04f62
record_format Article
spelling doaj-9c724507dda14d51a03cf8ddf3a04f622020-11-25T04:06:04ZengThe Royal SocietyRoyal Society Open Science2054-57032017-01-014110.1098/rsos.160661160661How do great bowerbirds construct perspective illusions?Laura A. KelleyJohn A. EndlerMany animals build structures to provide shelter, avoid predation, attract mates or house offspring, but the behaviour and potential cognitive processes involved during building are poorly understood. Great bowerbird (Ptilinorhynchus nuchalis) males build and maintain display courts by placing tens to hundreds of objects in a positive size–distance gradient. The visual angles created by the gradient create a forced perspective illusion that females can use to choose a mate. Although the quality of illusion is consistent within males, it varies among males, which may reflect differences in how individuals reconstruct their courts. We moved all objects off display courts to determine how males reconstructed the visual illusion. We found that all individuals rapidly created the positive size–distance gradient required for forced perspective within the first 10 objects placed. Males began court reconstruction by placing objects in the centre of the court and then placing objects further out, a technique commonly used when humans lay mosaics. The number of objects present after 72 h was not related to mating success or the quality of the illusion, indicating that male skill at arranging objects rather than absolute number of objects appears to be important. We conclude that differences arise in the quality of forced perspective illusions despite males using the same technique to reconstruct their courts.https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/pdf/10.1098/rsos.160661bowerbirdconstruction behaviourforced perspective
collection DOAJ
language English
format Article
sources DOAJ
author Laura A. Kelley
John A. Endler
spellingShingle Laura A. Kelley
John A. Endler
How do great bowerbirds construct perspective illusions?
Royal Society Open Science
bowerbird
construction behaviour
forced perspective
author_facet Laura A. Kelley
John A. Endler
author_sort Laura A. Kelley
title How do great bowerbirds construct perspective illusions?
title_short How do great bowerbirds construct perspective illusions?
title_full How do great bowerbirds construct perspective illusions?
title_fullStr How do great bowerbirds construct perspective illusions?
title_full_unstemmed How do great bowerbirds construct perspective illusions?
title_sort how do great bowerbirds construct perspective illusions?
publisher The Royal Society
series Royal Society Open Science
issn 2054-5703
publishDate 2017-01-01
description Many animals build structures to provide shelter, avoid predation, attract mates or house offspring, but the behaviour and potential cognitive processes involved during building are poorly understood. Great bowerbird (Ptilinorhynchus nuchalis) males build and maintain display courts by placing tens to hundreds of objects in a positive size–distance gradient. The visual angles created by the gradient create a forced perspective illusion that females can use to choose a mate. Although the quality of illusion is consistent within males, it varies among males, which may reflect differences in how individuals reconstruct their courts. We moved all objects off display courts to determine how males reconstructed the visual illusion. We found that all individuals rapidly created the positive size–distance gradient required for forced perspective within the first 10 objects placed. Males began court reconstruction by placing objects in the centre of the court and then placing objects further out, a technique commonly used when humans lay mosaics. The number of objects present after 72 h was not related to mating success or the quality of the illusion, indicating that male skill at arranging objects rather than absolute number of objects appears to be important. We conclude that differences arise in the quality of forced perspective illusions despite males using the same technique to reconstruct their courts.
topic bowerbird
construction behaviour
forced perspective
url https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/pdf/10.1098/rsos.160661
work_keys_str_mv AT lauraakelley howdogreatbowerbirdsconstructperspectiveillusions
AT johnaendler howdogreatbowerbirdsconstructperspectiveillusions
_version_ 1724432592652992512