Which way is down? Positional distortion in the tilt illusion.

Contextual information can have a huge impact on our sensory experience. The tilt illusion is a classic example of contextual influence exerted by an oriented surround on a target's perceived orientation. Traditionally, the tilt illusion has been described as the outcome of inhibition between c...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Alessandro Tomassini, Joshua Adam Solomon, Michael John Morgan
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Public Library of Science (PLoS) 2014-01-01
Series:PLoS ONE
Online Access:http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC4208767?pdf=render
id doaj-d2fc84eeb3ef440b9f146e4f21decde2
record_format Article
spelling doaj-d2fc84eeb3ef440b9f146e4f21decde22020-11-25T01:20:49ZengPublic Library of Science (PLoS)PLoS ONE1932-62032014-01-01910e11072910.1371/journal.pone.0110729Which way is down? Positional distortion in the tilt illusion.Alessandro TomassiniJoshua Adam SolomonMichael John MorganContextual information can have a huge impact on our sensory experience. The tilt illusion is a classic example of contextual influence exerted by an oriented surround on a target's perceived orientation. Traditionally, the tilt illusion has been described as the outcome of inhibition between cortical neurons with adjacent receptive fields and a similar preference for orientation. An alternative explanation is that tilted contexts could produce a re-calibration of the subjective frame of reference. Although the distinction is subtle, only the latter model makes clear predictions for unoriented stimuli. In the present study, we tested one such prediction by asking four naive subjects to estimate three positions (4, 6, and 8 o'clock) on an imaginary clock face within a tilted surround. To indicate their estimates, they used either an unoriented dot or a line segment, with one endpoint at fixation in the middle of the surround. The surround's tilt was randomly chosen from a set of orientations (± 75°, ± 65°, ± 55°, ± 45°, ± 35°, ± 25°, ± 15°, ± 5° with respect to vertical) across trials. Our results showed systematic biases consistent with the tilt illusion in both conditions. Biases were largest when observers attempted to estimate the 4 and 8 o'clock positions, but there was no significant difference between data gathered with the dot and data gathered with the line segment. A control experiment confirmed that biases were better accounted for by a local coordinate shift than to torsional eye movements induced by the tilted context. This finding supports the idea that tilted contexts distort perceived positions as well as perceived orientations and cannot be readily explained by lateral interactions between orientation selective cells in V1.http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC4208767?pdf=render
collection DOAJ
language English
format Article
sources DOAJ
author Alessandro Tomassini
Joshua Adam Solomon
Michael John Morgan
spellingShingle Alessandro Tomassini
Joshua Adam Solomon
Michael John Morgan
Which way is down? Positional distortion in the tilt illusion.
PLoS ONE
author_facet Alessandro Tomassini
Joshua Adam Solomon
Michael John Morgan
author_sort Alessandro Tomassini
title Which way is down? Positional distortion in the tilt illusion.
title_short Which way is down? Positional distortion in the tilt illusion.
title_full Which way is down? Positional distortion in the tilt illusion.
title_fullStr Which way is down? Positional distortion in the tilt illusion.
title_full_unstemmed Which way is down? Positional distortion in the tilt illusion.
title_sort which way is down? positional distortion in the tilt illusion.
publisher Public Library of Science (PLoS)
series PLoS ONE
issn 1932-6203
publishDate 2014-01-01
description Contextual information can have a huge impact on our sensory experience. The tilt illusion is a classic example of contextual influence exerted by an oriented surround on a target's perceived orientation. Traditionally, the tilt illusion has been described as the outcome of inhibition between cortical neurons with adjacent receptive fields and a similar preference for orientation. An alternative explanation is that tilted contexts could produce a re-calibration of the subjective frame of reference. Although the distinction is subtle, only the latter model makes clear predictions for unoriented stimuli. In the present study, we tested one such prediction by asking four naive subjects to estimate three positions (4, 6, and 8 o'clock) on an imaginary clock face within a tilted surround. To indicate their estimates, they used either an unoriented dot or a line segment, with one endpoint at fixation in the middle of the surround. The surround's tilt was randomly chosen from a set of orientations (± 75°, ± 65°, ± 55°, ± 45°, ± 35°, ± 25°, ± 15°, ± 5° with respect to vertical) across trials. Our results showed systematic biases consistent with the tilt illusion in both conditions. Biases were largest when observers attempted to estimate the 4 and 8 o'clock positions, but there was no significant difference between data gathered with the dot and data gathered with the line segment. A control experiment confirmed that biases were better accounted for by a local coordinate shift than to torsional eye movements induced by the tilted context. This finding supports the idea that tilted contexts distort perceived positions as well as perceived orientations and cannot be readily explained by lateral interactions between orientation selective cells in V1.
url http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC4208767?pdf=render
work_keys_str_mv AT alessandrotomassini whichwayisdownpositionaldistortioninthetiltillusion
AT joshuaadamsolomon whichwayisdownpositionaldistortioninthetiltillusion
AT michaeljohnmorgan whichwayisdownpositionaldistortioninthetiltillusion
_version_ 1725131807435784192