Cognitive control in Russian-German bilinguals

Multilingual speakers are faced with the problem to keep their languages apart, but do so with interindividually varying success. Cognitive control abilities might be an important factor to explain such interindividual differences. Here we compare two late, balanced and highly proficient bilingual g...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Julia eFestman, Thomas F Münte
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Frontiers Media S.A. 2012-04-01
Series:Frontiers in Psychology
Subjects:
ERN
Online Access:http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00115/full
id doaj-fc02f0984ed146289f0705f97537ed52
record_format Article
spelling doaj-fc02f0984ed146289f0705f97537ed522020-11-25T00:59:34ZengFrontiers Media S.A.Frontiers in Psychology1664-10782012-04-01310.3389/fpsyg.2012.0011510275Cognitive control in Russian-German bilingualsJulia eFestman0Thomas F Münte1University of PotsdamUniversity of LübeckMultilingual speakers are faced with the problem to keep their languages apart, but do so with interindividually varying success. Cognitive control abilities might be an important factor to explain such interindividual differences. Here we compare two late, balanced and highly proficient bilingual groups (mean age 24 years, L1 Russian, L2 German) which were established according to their language control abilities on a bilingual picture-naming task. One group had difficulties to remain in the instructed target language and switched unintentionally to the non-target language (switchers), whereas the other group rarely switched unintentionally (non-switchers). This group-specific behaviour could not be explained by language background, socio-cultural or demographic variables. Rather, the non-switchers also demonstrated a faster and better performance on four cognitive control tests (Tower of Hanoi, Ruff Figural Fluency Test, Divided Attention, Go/Nogo). In this paper we will focus on two additional cognitive tasks, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) and the Flanker task. Importantly, these two tasks require executive processing components for conflict monitoring and conflict resolution. Non-switchers outperformed switchers with regard to speed and accuracy, and were better at finding and applying the correct rules in the WCST. On the Flanker task, non-switchers performed faster and better on conflict trials and had a higher correction rate following an error. Event-related potential recordings furthermore revealed a smaller error-related negativity in the non-switchers. In sum, the non-switcher group consistently performed better and faster, in particular on high-conflict trials, probably due to better efficiency in dealing with conflict. They also demonstrated overall better self-monitoring abilities compared to switchers. We conclude that bilingual language performance, in particular switching behavior is related to performance on cognitive control tasks.http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00115/fullflanker taskinhibitionSelf-MonitoringConflict ResolutionERNinformation processing advantage
collection DOAJ
language English
format Article
sources DOAJ
author Julia eFestman
Thomas F Münte
spellingShingle Julia eFestman
Thomas F Münte
Cognitive control in Russian-German bilinguals
Frontiers in Psychology
flanker task
inhibition
Self-Monitoring
Conflict Resolution
ERN
information processing advantage
author_facet Julia eFestman
Thomas F Münte
author_sort Julia eFestman
title Cognitive control in Russian-German bilinguals
title_short Cognitive control in Russian-German bilinguals
title_full Cognitive control in Russian-German bilinguals
title_fullStr Cognitive control in Russian-German bilinguals
title_full_unstemmed Cognitive control in Russian-German bilinguals
title_sort cognitive control in russian-german bilinguals
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
series Frontiers in Psychology
issn 1664-1078
publishDate 2012-04-01
description Multilingual speakers are faced with the problem to keep their languages apart, but do so with interindividually varying success. Cognitive control abilities might be an important factor to explain such interindividual differences. Here we compare two late, balanced and highly proficient bilingual groups (mean age 24 years, L1 Russian, L2 German) which were established according to their language control abilities on a bilingual picture-naming task. One group had difficulties to remain in the instructed target language and switched unintentionally to the non-target language (switchers), whereas the other group rarely switched unintentionally (non-switchers). This group-specific behaviour could not be explained by language background, socio-cultural or demographic variables. Rather, the non-switchers also demonstrated a faster and better performance on four cognitive control tests (Tower of Hanoi, Ruff Figural Fluency Test, Divided Attention, Go/Nogo). In this paper we will focus on two additional cognitive tasks, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) and the Flanker task. Importantly, these two tasks require executive processing components for conflict monitoring and conflict resolution. Non-switchers outperformed switchers with regard to speed and accuracy, and were better at finding and applying the correct rules in the WCST. On the Flanker task, non-switchers performed faster and better on conflict trials and had a higher correction rate following an error. Event-related potential recordings furthermore revealed a smaller error-related negativity in the non-switchers. In sum, the non-switcher group consistently performed better and faster, in particular on high-conflict trials, probably due to better efficiency in dealing with conflict. They also demonstrated overall better self-monitoring abilities compared to switchers. We conclude that bilingual language performance, in particular switching behavior is related to performance on cognitive control tasks.
topic flanker task
inhibition
Self-Monitoring
Conflict Resolution
ERN
information processing advantage
url http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00115/full
work_keys_str_mv AT juliaefestman cognitivecontrolinrussiangermanbilinguals
AT thomasfmunte cognitivecontrolinrussiangermanbilinguals
_version_ 1725217678323351552