Infant Effects on Experimenter Behavior

Beginning in the late 1960's (e.g., Bell, 1968), a considerable literature has emerged documenting the impact of children's characteristics on their own care and biopsychosocial outcomes. Yet, surprisingly little research has focused on the impact of the child on the experimental setting....

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Dixon, Wallace E., Jr., Driggers-Jones, Lauren P., Robertson, Chelsea L.
Published: Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University 2018
Subjects:
Online Access:https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/4906
id ndltd-ETSU-oai-dc.etsu.edu-etsu-works-6108
record_format oai_dc
spelling ndltd-ETSU-oai-dc.etsu.edu-etsu-works-61082019-10-19T03:30:48Z Infant Effects on Experimenter Behavior Dixon, Wallace E., Jr. Driggers-Jones, Lauren P. Robertson, Chelsea L. Beginning in the late 1960's (e.g., Bell, 1968), a considerable literature has emerged documenting the impact of children's characteristics on their own care and biopsychosocial outcomes. Yet, surprisingly little research has focused on the impact of the child on the experimental setting. It is well known in the infant literature that infant emotional states contribute to their own attrition, and even cognitive performance (e.g., Fagen et al., 1991). Less well known is the extent that infant characteristics contribute to experimenter social engagement. In the present investigation, we explored whether two experimenters responded to infants differently as a function of infant temperament. Sixty- 334 five infants (37 girls) visited the lab at M = 15.38 months (SD = 1.99). Mothers completed the Infant Behavior Questionnaire - Revised (IBQ-R) and a demographic assessment. Temperament measures derived from the IBQ-R were reduced to three overarching superdimensions (negative affectivity, effortful control, and surgency) from 14 subdimensions. Infants participated in a Brooks and Meltzoff (2005) type gaze-following procedure. On Trial 1, either of two experimenters sitting directly across from the infant established eye-contact by calling the child's name, said "Look!", then turned their head to look at a target object on the infant's left for 8 seconds. On Trial 2, experimenters followed the same procedure but looked to the infant's right. Trial 3 was the same as Trial 1. On Trials 4-6, experimenters followed a right-left-right pattern, with the exception that an Elmo videotape played on a monitor behind and above the experimenter as soon as the experimenter looked at the target object. Trials 4-6 were designed to test gazefollowing under conditions of distraction. The two experimenters did not differ statistically from one another in looking to the target object on any trial (see Table 1; t's <= 1.60, p's => .12); although, due to procedural requirements looking time for both experimenters differed as a function of distraction condition [F(1, 57) = 98.53, p = .000; see Table 1]. Nevertheless, during a procedural fidelity check, and despite both experimenters being blind to children's temperamental status, we found that experimenter looking time to the target objects in the control condition was correlated with both effortful control and surgency (see Table 2). These correlations were carried primarily by the subdimensions of duration of orientation and perceptual sensitivity, respectively. Evaluating the correlations separately by experimenter showed that both experimenters appeared to be susceptible to infant temperament. These results raise the possibility that even highly trained experimenters, blind to child temperament status, may be responsive to child characteristics when implementing experimental protocols. Obviously, in the present case, when experimenters remained visually engaged with target objects for longer periods of time for certain children, those children had greater opportunity to demonstrate gazefollowing. In principle, children high in effortful control and surgency could demonstrate longer gaze-following not as a direct effect of their temperament, but as an indirect effect of their temperament mediated through an experimenter. Future experimental researchers may wish to include temperament instruments as standard protocol to test for experimenter fidelity. 2018-07-01T07:00:00Z text https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/4906 ETSU Faculty Works Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University infants experimenter behavior Psychology Maternal Child Health Child Psychology Developmental Psychology
collection NDLTD
sources NDLTD
topic infants
experimenter behavior
Psychology
Maternal Child Health
Child Psychology
Developmental Psychology
spellingShingle infants
experimenter behavior
Psychology
Maternal Child Health
Child Psychology
Developmental Psychology
Dixon, Wallace E., Jr.
Driggers-Jones, Lauren P.
Robertson, Chelsea L.
Infant Effects on Experimenter Behavior
description Beginning in the late 1960's (e.g., Bell, 1968), a considerable literature has emerged documenting the impact of children's characteristics on their own care and biopsychosocial outcomes. Yet, surprisingly little research has focused on the impact of the child on the experimental setting. It is well known in the infant literature that infant emotional states contribute to their own attrition, and even cognitive performance (e.g., Fagen et al., 1991). Less well known is the extent that infant characteristics contribute to experimenter social engagement. In the present investigation, we explored whether two experimenters responded to infants differently as a function of infant temperament. Sixty- 334 five infants (37 girls) visited the lab at M = 15.38 months (SD = 1.99). Mothers completed the Infant Behavior Questionnaire - Revised (IBQ-R) and a demographic assessment. Temperament measures derived from the IBQ-R were reduced to three overarching superdimensions (negative affectivity, effortful control, and surgency) from 14 subdimensions. Infants participated in a Brooks and Meltzoff (2005) type gaze-following procedure. On Trial 1, either of two experimenters sitting directly across from the infant established eye-contact by calling the child's name, said "Look!", then turned their head to look at a target object on the infant's left for 8 seconds. On Trial 2, experimenters followed the same procedure but looked to the infant's right. Trial 3 was the same as Trial 1. On Trials 4-6, experimenters followed a right-left-right pattern, with the exception that an Elmo videotape played on a monitor behind and above the experimenter as soon as the experimenter looked at the target object. Trials 4-6 were designed to test gazefollowing under conditions of distraction. The two experimenters did not differ statistically from one another in looking to the target object on any trial (see Table 1; t's <= 1.60, p's => .12); although, due to procedural requirements looking time for both experimenters differed as a function of distraction condition [F(1, 57) = 98.53, p = .000; see Table 1]. Nevertheless, during a procedural fidelity check, and despite both experimenters being blind to children's temperamental status, we found that experimenter looking time to the target objects in the control condition was correlated with both effortful control and surgency (see Table 2). These correlations were carried primarily by the subdimensions of duration of orientation and perceptual sensitivity, respectively. Evaluating the correlations separately by experimenter showed that both experimenters appeared to be susceptible to infant temperament. These results raise the possibility that even highly trained experimenters, blind to child temperament status, may be responsive to child characteristics when implementing experimental protocols. Obviously, in the present case, when experimenters remained visually engaged with target objects for longer periods of time for certain children, those children had greater opportunity to demonstrate gazefollowing. In principle, children high in effortful control and surgency could demonstrate longer gaze-following not as a direct effect of their temperament, but as an indirect effect of their temperament mediated through an experimenter. Future experimental researchers may wish to include temperament instruments as standard protocol to test for experimenter fidelity.
author Dixon, Wallace E., Jr.
Driggers-Jones, Lauren P.
Robertson, Chelsea L.
author_facet Dixon, Wallace E., Jr.
Driggers-Jones, Lauren P.
Robertson, Chelsea L.
author_sort Dixon, Wallace E., Jr.
title Infant Effects on Experimenter Behavior
title_short Infant Effects on Experimenter Behavior
title_full Infant Effects on Experimenter Behavior
title_fullStr Infant Effects on Experimenter Behavior
title_full_unstemmed Infant Effects on Experimenter Behavior
title_sort infant effects on experimenter behavior
publisher Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University
publishDate 2018
url https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/4906
work_keys_str_mv AT dixonwallaceejr infanteffectsonexperimenterbehavior
AT driggersjoneslaurenp infanteffectsonexperimenterbehavior
AT robertsonchelseal infanteffectsonexperimenterbehavior
_version_ 1719270166160736256