Using what's there: Bilinguals adaptively rely on orthographic and color cues to achieve language control

We examined if bilinguals of two different language combinations can rely on novel and arbitrary cues to facilitate switching between languages in a read-aloud task. Spanish-English (Experiment 1) and Hebrew-English (Experiment 2) bilinguals read aloud mixed-language paragraphs, known to induce lang...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Fadlon, J. (Author), Gollan, T.H (Author), Li, C. (Author), Prior, A. (Author)
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Elsevier B.V. 2019
Subjects:
Online Access:View Fulltext in Publisher
LEADER 02989nam a2200541Ia 4500
001 10.1016-j.cognition.2019.06.002
008 220511s2019 CNT 000 0 und d
020 |a 00100277 (ISSN) 
245 1 0 |a Using what's there: Bilinguals adaptively rely on orthographic and color cues to achieve language control 
260 0 |b Elsevier B.V.  |c 2019 
856 |z View Fulltext in Publisher  |u https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.06.002 
520 3 |a We examined if bilinguals of two different language combinations can rely on novel and arbitrary cues to facilitate switching between languages in a read-aloud task. Spanish-English (Experiment 1) and Hebrew-English (Experiment 2) bilinguals read aloud mixed-language paragraphs, known to induce language intrusion errors (e.g., saying el instead of the), to test if intrusion rates are affected by: language combination, color-cues, language dominance, and part of speech. For Spanish-English bilinguals, written input is not rich in visual cues to language membership, whereas for Hebrew-English bilinguals rich cues are present (i.e., the two languages have different orthographies and are read in opposite directions). Hebrew-English bilinguals made fewer intrusion errors than Spanish-English bilinguals, and color cues significantly reduced intrusions on switches to the dominant language but not to the nondominant language, to the same extent in both bilingual populations. These results reveal powerful effects of visual cues for facilitating production of language switches, and illustrate that switching mechanisms are highly adaptable and sensitive, in that they can both recruit language- and orthography-specific cues when available and also rapidly exploit novel arbitrary cues to language membership when these are afforded. Finally, such incidental, experimentally induced cues, were recruited even in the presence of other already powerful cues, when task demands were high. © 2019 Elsevier B.V. 
650 0 4 |a Adaptability 
650 0 4 |a adult 
650 0 4 |a Adult 
650 0 4 |a article 
650 0 4 |a association 
650 0 4 |a bilingualism 
650 0 4 |a Bilingualism 
650 0 4 |a Code-switching 
650 0 4 |a Color Perception 
650 0 4 |a color vision 
650 0 4 |a Cues 
650 0 4 |a error 
650 0 4 |a executive function 
650 0 4 |a Executive Function 
650 0 4 |a human 
650 0 4 |a human experiment 
650 0 4 |a Humans 
650 0 4 |a language 
650 0 4 |a Language control 
650 0 4 |a multilingualism 
650 0 4 |a Multilingualism 
650 0 4 |a pattern recognition 
650 0 4 |a Pattern Recognition, Visual 
650 0 4 |a physiology 
650 0 4 |a Production 
650 0 4 |a psycholinguistics 
650 0 4 |a Psycholinguistics 
650 0 4 |a reading 
650 0 4 |a Reading 
650 0 4 |a speech 
650 0 4 |a Visual cues 
700 1 |a Fadlon, J.  |e author 
700 1 |a Gollan, T.H.  |e author 
700 1 |a Li, C.  |e author 
700 1 |a Prior, A.  |e author 
773 |t Cognition