|
|
|
|
LEADER |
03397nam a2200649Ia 4500 |
001 |
10.1016-j.cognition.2021.104617 |
008 |
220427s2021 CNT 000 0 und d |
020 |
|
|
|a 00100277 (ISSN)
|
245 |
1 |
0 |
|a Crossmodal spatial distraction across the lifespan
|
260 |
|
0 |
|b Elsevier B.V.
|c 2021
|
856 |
|
|
|z View Fulltext in Publisher
|u https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104617
|
520 |
3 |
|
|a The ability to resist distracting stimuli whilst voluntarily focusing on a task is fundamental to our everyday cognitive functioning. Here, we investigated how this ability develops, and thereafter declines, across the lifespan using a single task/experiment. Young children (5–7 years), older children (10–11 years), young adults (20–27 years), and older adults (62–86 years) were presented with complex visual scenes. Endogenous (voluntary) attention was engaged by having the participants search for a visual target presented on either the left or right side of the display. The onset of the visual scenes was preceded – at stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) of 50, 200, or 500 ms – by a task-irrelevant sound (an exogenous crossmodal spatial distractor) delivered either on the same or opposite side as the visual target, or simultaneously on both sides (cued, uncued, or neutral trials, respectively). Age-related differences were revealed, especially in the extreme age-groups, which showed a greater impact of crossmodal spatial distractors. Young children were highly susceptible to exogenous spatial distraction at the shortest SOA (50 ms), whereas older adults were distracted at all SOAs, showing significant exogenous capture effects during the visual search task. By contrast, older children and young adults' search performance was not significantly affected by crossmodal spatial distraction. Overall, these findings present a detailed picture of the developmental trajectory of endogenous resistance to crossmodal spatial distraction from childhood to old age and demonstrate a different efficiency in coping with distraction across the four age-groups studied. © 2021 Elsevier B.V.
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a adolescent
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Adolescent
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a adult
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a aged
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Aged
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a article
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a association
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a attention
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Attention
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a child
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Child
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Child, Preschool
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a childhood
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a cognitive development
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Cognitive development
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Complex scenes
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Crossmodal spatial attention
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Cues
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a female
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a groups by age
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a human
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a human experiment
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Humans
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a lifespan
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a longevity
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Longevity
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a male
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Photic Stimulation
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a photostimulation
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a preschool child
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a reaction time
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Reaction Time
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a school child
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a sound
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Stimulus onset asynchrony
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Visual search
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a young adult
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Young Adult
|
700 |
1 |
|
|a Bremner, A.J.
|e author
|
700 |
1 |
|
|a Capurso, M.
|e author
|
700 |
1 |
|
|a Mastroberardino, S.
|e author
|
700 |
1 |
|
|a Pedale, T.
|e author
|
700 |
1 |
|
|a Santangelo, V.
|e author
|
700 |
1 |
|
|a Spence, C.
|e author
|
773 |
|
|
|t Cognition
|