Controlling the narrative: Euphemistic language affects judgments of actions while avoiding perceptions of dishonesty

The present work (N = 1906 U.S. residents) investigates the extent to which peoples' evaluations of actions can be biased by the strategic use of euphemistic (agreeable) and dysphemistic (disagreeable) terms. We find that participants' evaluations of actions are made more favorable by repl...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Fugelsang, J.A (Author), Koehler, D.J (Author), Meyers, E.A (Author), Stolz, J.A (Author), Turpin, M.H (Author), Walker, A.C (Author)
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Elsevier B.V. 2021
Subjects:
Online Access:View Fulltext in Publisher
LEADER 02849nam a2200553Ia 4500
001 10.1016-j.cognition.2021.104633
008 220427s2021 CNT 000 0 und d
020 |a 00100277 (ISSN) 
245 1 0 |a Controlling the narrative: Euphemistic language affects judgments of actions while avoiding perceptions of dishonesty 
260 0 |b Elsevier B.V.  |c 2021 
856 |z View Fulltext in Publisher  |u https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104633 
520 3 |a The present work (N = 1906 U.S. residents) investigates the extent to which peoples' evaluations of actions can be biased by the strategic use of euphemistic (agreeable) and dysphemistic (disagreeable) terms. We find that participants' evaluations of actions are made more favorable by replacing a disagreeable term (e.g., torture) with a semantically related agreeable term (e.g., enhanced interrogation) in an act's description. Notably, the influence of agreeable and disagreeable terms was reduced (but not eliminated) when making actions less ambiguous by providing participants with a detailed description of each action. Despite their influence, participants judged both agreeable and disagreeable action descriptions as largely truthful and distinct from lies, and judged agents using such descriptions as more trustworthy and moral than liars. Overall, the results of the current study suggest that a strategic speaker can, through the careful use of language, sway the opinions of others in a preferred direction while avoiding many of the reputational costs associated with less subtle forms of linguistic manipulation (e.g., lying). Like the much-studied phenomenon of “fake news,” manipulative language can serve as a tool for misleading the public, doing so not with falsehoods but rather the strategic use of language. © 2021 The Author(s) 
650 0 4 |a adult 
650 0 4 |a article 
650 0 4 |a deception 
650 0 4 |a deception 
650 0 4 |a Deception 
650 0 4 |a Deception 
650 0 4 |a decision making 
650 0 4 |a disinformation 
650 0 4 |a Doublespeak 
650 0 4 |a Euphemism 
650 0 4 |a female 
650 0 4 |a human 
650 0 4 |a human experiment 
650 0 4 |a Humans 
650 0 4 |a Judgment 
650 0 4 |a language 
650 0 4 |a Language 
650 0 4 |a Linguistic framing 
650 0 4 |a Linguistic manipulation 
650 0 4 |a major clinical study 
650 0 4 |a male 
650 0 4 |a morality 
650 0 4 |a morality 
650 0 4 |a Morals 
650 0 4 |a narrative 
650 0 4 |a perception 
650 0 4 |a perception 
650 0 4 |a Perception 
650 0 4 |a resident 
650 0 4 |a torture 
700 1 |a Fugelsang, J.A.  |e author 
700 1 |a Koehler, D.J.  |e author 
700 1 |a Meyers, E.A.  |e author 
700 1 |a Stolz, J.A.  |e author 
700 1 |a Turpin, M.H.  |e author 
700 1 |a Walker, A.C.  |e author 
773 |t Cognition