Exploiting failures in metacognition through magic: Visual awareness as a source of visual metacognition bias

We used cognitive illusions/magic tricks to study the role of visual awareness as a source of biases in visual metacognitive judgments. We conducted a questionnaire-based study (n = 144) and an eye tracking study (n = 69) in which participants watched videos of four different magic tricks that capit...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Barnhart, A. (Author), Kuhn, G. (Author), Montañes, P. (Author), Ortega, J. (Author)
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Academic Press Inc. 2018
Subjects:
Online Access:View Fulltext in Publisher
LEADER 02995nam a2200685Ia 4500
001 10.1016-j.concog.2018.08.008
008 220706s2018 CNT 000 0 und d
020 |a 10538100 (ISSN) 
245 1 0 |a Exploiting failures in metacognition through magic: Visual awareness as a source of visual metacognition bias 
260 0 |b Academic Press Inc.  |c 2018 
856 |z View Fulltext in Publisher  |u https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2018.08.008 
520 3 |a We used cognitive illusions/magic tricks to study the role of visual awareness as a source of biases in visual metacognitive judgments. We conducted a questionnaire-based study (n = 144) and an eye tracking study (n = 69) in which participants watched videos of four different magic tricks that capitalize on failures of visual awareness (inattentional blindness and change blindness). We measured participants’ susceptibility to these illusions, their beliefs about other people's susceptibility, as well as the role that fixating (i.e. eye position) the critical event has on detecting the secret. Participants who detected the method of the tricks believed it was more likely that other people would detect it compared to those participants who failed to notice the method. Moreover, they believed that they moved their eyes to look at it. Eye tracking data show that, contrary to participants’ beliefs, peripheral vision played a significant role in detecting the method. Overall, the findings from these studies suggest that visual awareness may bias visual metacognitive judgments. © 2018 Elsevier Inc. 
650 0 4 |a adult 
650 0 4 |a Adult 
650 0 4 |a Article 
650 0 4 |a awareness 
650 0 4 |a Awareness 
650 0 4 |a change blindness 
650 0 4 |a cognitive bias 
650 0 4 |a Cognitive illusions 
650 0 4 |a controlled study 
650 0 4 |a decision making 
650 0 4 |a Eye Movement Measurements 
650 0 4 |a Eye movements 
650 0 4 |a eye tracking 
650 0 4 |a female 
650 0 4 |a Female 
650 0 4 |a human 
650 0 4 |a human experiment 
650 0 4 |a Humans 
650 0 4 |a illusion 
650 0 4 |a Illusions 
650 0 4 |a inattentional blindness 
650 0 4 |a magic 
650 0 4 |a male 
650 0 4 |a Male 
650 0 4 |a metacognition 
650 0 4 |a Metacognition 
650 0 4 |a metacognitive awareness 
650 0 4 |a Metacognitive biases 
650 0 4 |a normal human 
650 0 4 |a oculography 
650 0 4 |a perception 
650 0 4 |a peripheral vision 
650 0 4 |a physiology 
650 0 4 |a psychophysiology 
650 0 4 |a questionnaire 
650 0 4 |a Social Perception 
650 0 4 |a videorecording 
650 0 4 |a Visual awareness 
650 0 4 |a visual illusion 
650 0 4 |a Visual metacognition 
650 0 4 |a visual stimulation 
650 0 4 |a young adult 
650 0 4 |a Young Adult 
700 1 |a Barnhart, A.  |e author 
700 1 |a Kuhn, G.  |e author 
700 1 |a Montañes, P.  |e author 
700 1 |a Ortega, J.  |e author 
773 |t Consciousness and Cognition