Vocal and tactile input to children who are deaf or hard of hearing

Purpose: Caregivers may show greater use of nonauditory signals in interactions with children who are deaf or hard of hearing (DHH). This study explored the frequency of maternal touch and the temporal alignment of touch with speech in the input to children who are DHH and age-matched peers with nor...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Abu-Zhaya, R. (Author), Houston, D. (Author), Kondaurov, M.V (Author), Seidl, A. (Author)
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 2019
Subjects:
Online Access:View Fulltext in Publisher
LEADER 02864nam a2200589Ia 4500
001 10.1044-2019_JSLHR-L-18-0185
008 220511s2019 CNT 000 0 und d
020 |a 10924388 (ISSN) 
245 1 0 |a Vocal and tactile input to children who are deaf or hard of hearing 
260 0 |b American Speech-Language-Hearing Association  |c 2019 
856 |z View Fulltext in Publisher  |u https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-L-18-0185 
520 3 |a Purpose: Caregivers may show greater use of nonauditory signals in interactions with children who are deaf or hard of hearing (DHH). This study explored the frequency of maternal touch and the temporal alignment of touch with speech in the input to children who are DHH and age-matched peers with normal hearing. Method: We gathered audio and video recordings of mother– child free-play interactions. Maternal speech units were annotated from audio recordings, and touch events were annotated from video recordings. Analyses explored the frequency and duration of touch events and the temporal alignment of touch with speech. Results: Greater variance was observed in the frequency of touch and its total duration in the input to children who are DHH. Furthermore, touches produced by mothers of children who are DHH were significantly more likely to be aligned with speech than touches produced by mothers of children with normal hearing. Conclusion: Caregivers’ modifications in the input to children who are DHH are observed in the combination of speech with touch. The implications for such patterns and how they may impact children’s attention and access to the speech signal are discussed. © 2019 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. 
650 0 4 |a article 
650 0 4 |a attention 
650 0 4 |a audio recording 
650 0 4 |a caregiver 
650 0 4 |a child 
650 0 4 |a Child, Preschool 
650 0 4 |a controlled study 
650 0 4 |a Deafness 
650 0 4 |a female 
650 0 4 |a Female 
650 0 4 |a hearing impaired person 
650 0 4 |a hearing impaired person 
650 0 4 |a hearing impairment 
650 0 4 |a Hearing Loss 
650 0 4 |a human 
650 0 4 |a Humans 
650 0 4 |a infant 
650 0 4 |a Infant 
650 0 4 |a language development 
650 0 4 |a Language Development 
650 0 4 |a male 
650 0 4 |a Male 
650 0 4 |a mother 
650 0 4 |a pathophysiology 
650 0 4 |a Persons With Hearing Impairments 
650 0 4 |a physiology 
650 0 4 |a preschool child 
650 0 4 |a psychology 
650 0 4 |a speech 
650 0 4 |a Speech 
650 0 4 |a time factor 
650 0 4 |a Time Factors 
650 0 4 |a touch 
650 0 4 |a Touch 
650 0 4 |a videorecording 
700 1 |a Abu-Zhaya, R.  |e author 
700 1 |a Houston, D.  |e author 
700 1 |a Kondaurov, M.V.  |e author 
700 1 |a Seidl, A.  |e author 
773 |t Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research