Word-learning strategies and mean length of utterances in Mandarin-English bilingual children

碩士 === 朝陽科技大學 === 幼兒保育系碩士班 === 93 === In this study, we set three experiments on detecting Mandarin-English bilingual children and monolingual children’s use of word-learning strategies and mean length of utterances. In these experiments, we use 4-year-old and 6-year-old Mandarin-English bilingual a...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Li-Chin Hsieh, 謝莉卿
Other Authors: Kuo-You Huang
Format: Others
Language:zh-TW
Published: 2005
Online Access:http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/70577804085839889263
id ndltd-TW-093CYUT5709004
record_format oai_dc
spelling ndltd-TW-093CYUT57090042015-10-13T11:54:00Z http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/70577804085839889263 Word-learning strategies and mean length of utterances in Mandarin-English bilingual children 國英雙語幼兒詞彙學習策略與平均說話長度 Li-Chin Hsieh 謝莉卿 碩士 朝陽科技大學 幼兒保育系碩士班 93 In this study, we set three experiments on detecting Mandarin-English bilingual children and monolingual children’s use of word-learning strategies and mean length of utterances. In these experiments, we use 4-year-old and 6-year-old Mandarin-English bilingual and Mandarin monolinguals children to discuss these experiments. In the first experiment, we focus on the mutual exclusivity constraint across children’s word-learning and compare the different performance between two groups of children. We found that both monolinguals and bilingual children would choose the referent objects correctly when different language inputs and 6-year-old children tendency stronger to use the mutual exclusivity constraint than 4-year-old children. In the second experiment, we add social context in a sentence to investigate whether influence inference of the word-meaning in monolinguals and bilingual or not. We found that both monolinguals and bilingual children would choose the referent objects correctly when they encountered stronger referential intention. These results show that Mandarin-English bilingual and Mandarin monolinguals children would adopt both the mutual exclusivity constraint and social-pragmatic cues to interpret the meanings of new words. In the final experiment, we show picture to children and to detecting children’s mean length of utterances. Results indicate that performance of speech productivity in 6-year-old children are better then 4-year-old and 4-year-old Mandarin-English bilingual children’s MLU are better than 4-year-old Mandarin children’s, but 6-year-old Mandarin children’s MLU are better than 6-year-old Mandarin-English bilingual children''s. Kuo-You Huang 黃國祐 2005 學位論文 ; thesis 101 zh-TW
collection NDLTD
language zh-TW
format Others
sources NDLTD
description 碩士 === 朝陽科技大學 === 幼兒保育系碩士班 === 93 === In this study, we set three experiments on detecting Mandarin-English bilingual children and monolingual children’s use of word-learning strategies and mean length of utterances. In these experiments, we use 4-year-old and 6-year-old Mandarin-English bilingual and Mandarin monolinguals children to discuss these experiments. In the first experiment, we focus on the mutual exclusivity constraint across children’s word-learning and compare the different performance between two groups of children. We found that both monolinguals and bilingual children would choose the referent objects correctly when different language inputs and 6-year-old children tendency stronger to use the mutual exclusivity constraint than 4-year-old children. In the second experiment, we add social context in a sentence to investigate whether influence inference of the word-meaning in monolinguals and bilingual or not. We found that both monolinguals and bilingual children would choose the referent objects correctly when they encountered stronger referential intention. These results show that Mandarin-English bilingual and Mandarin monolinguals children would adopt both the mutual exclusivity constraint and social-pragmatic cues to interpret the meanings of new words. In the final experiment, we show picture to children and to detecting children’s mean length of utterances. Results indicate that performance of speech productivity in 6-year-old children are better then 4-year-old and 4-year-old Mandarin-English bilingual children’s MLU are better than 4-year-old Mandarin children’s, but 6-year-old Mandarin children’s MLU are better than 6-year-old Mandarin-English bilingual children''s.
author2 Kuo-You Huang
author_facet Kuo-You Huang
Li-Chin Hsieh
謝莉卿
author Li-Chin Hsieh
謝莉卿
spellingShingle Li-Chin Hsieh
謝莉卿
Word-learning strategies and mean length of utterances in Mandarin-English bilingual children
author_sort Li-Chin Hsieh
title Word-learning strategies and mean length of utterances in Mandarin-English bilingual children
title_short Word-learning strategies and mean length of utterances in Mandarin-English bilingual children
title_full Word-learning strategies and mean length of utterances in Mandarin-English bilingual children
title_fullStr Word-learning strategies and mean length of utterances in Mandarin-English bilingual children
title_full_unstemmed Word-learning strategies and mean length of utterances in Mandarin-English bilingual children
title_sort word-learning strategies and mean length of utterances in mandarin-english bilingual children
publishDate 2005
url http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/70577804085839889263
work_keys_str_mv AT lichinhsieh wordlearningstrategiesandmeanlengthofutterancesinmandarinenglishbilingualchildren
AT xièlìqīng wordlearningstrategiesandmeanlengthofutterancesinmandarinenglishbilingualchildren
AT lichinhsieh guóyīngshuāngyǔyòuércíhuìxuéxícèlüèyǔpíngjūnshuōhuàzhǎngdù
AT xièlìqīng guóyīngshuāngyǔyòuércíhuìxuéxícèlüèyǔpíngjūnshuōhuàzhǎngdù
_version_ 1716850462213275648